Thinking About Thinking (2024)

Thinking About Thinking (1)

Source: Copyright Randall Munroe (CC BY-NC 2.5)

How should we think about thinking? Is even trying to do this akin to trying to open a box with a crowbar locked inside it?

A widely-shared Aeon article from earlier this year got very angry and confused on the whole issue, concluding that the whole of cognitive science was based on a very simple error—that error being that “humans are computers”. (1) Needless to say the author, Robert Epstein, was very stern and sarcastic at the foolishness of the assertion that we are little clockwork toys beeping around mindlessly, and he was at pains to set us all right.

Unfortunately, in his eagerness to correct, Epstein showed his misunderstanding of science in general, cognitive science in particular, and the march of history into the bargain. The straw man he created has gone down, but nobody in science is going to care. Here is why:

Wheels within wheels

I’d like to deal with the history part first, because it’s something that lots of people don’t know but that I am lucky enough to benefit from directly. The usual story about the “humans as computers” metaphor goes like this:

Humans have always compared thinking to their most impressive technology, hydraulics or clockwork say, computers are just the latest in a long line of this. Every other metaphor has failed, so will this one.

Epstein puts it this way:

“In the earliest one, eventually preserved in the Bible, humans were formed from clay or dirt, which an intelligent god then infused with its spirit. That spirit ‘explained’ our intelligence – grammatically, at least.

The invention of hydraulic engineering in the 3rd century BCE led to the popularity of a hydraulic model of human intelligence, the idea that the flow of different fluids in the body – the ‘humours’ – accounted for both our physical and mental functioning…By the 1500s, automata powered by springs and gears had been devised, eventually inspiring leading thinkers such as René Descartes to assert that humans are complex machines. In the 1600s, the British philosopher Thomas Hobbes suggested that thinking arose from small mechanical motions in the brain. By the 1700s, discoveries about electricity and chemistry led to new theories of human intelligence – again, largely metaphorical in nature. In the mid-1800s, inspired by recent advances in communications, the German physicist Hermann von Helmholtz compared the brain to a telegraph. Each metaphor reflected the most advanced thinking of the era that spawned it. Predictably, just a few years after the dawn of computer technology in the 1940s, the brain was said to operate like a computer, with the role of physical hardware played by the brain itself and our thoughts serving as software. The landmark event that launched what is now broadly called ‘cognitive science’ was the publication of Language and Communication (1951) by the psychologist George Miller.”

Sorry for the long quote—but actually Epstein gives a pretty good survey of the ways that humans have tried to use metaphors to explain human cognition. And it's a characterization that even Epstein's opponents like Sam Harris and David Krakauer agree with.(2) However, this history of humans as "epistemological narcissists", who project their latest technology onto themselves has one rather glaring omission. Which brings me back to the personal remark at the start. The person missing from this story is the person whose home I can see if I lean out of my office window dangerously far, whose name adorns the lecture halls I teach in and the library I study in. His work, and that of his equally erudite wife, is the major reason for the existence of the machine on which I write this and the reason you can read it. His name is George Boole, and the insights he had—in attempting to analyze all cognition--fully one hundred years before the invention of the physical computer, are the reason computers exist in the first place. (3).

Thinking About Thinking (2)

George Boole.

Source: copyright UCC (with permission)

Coming to the Boole

There isn’t the space to go into detail here but it is Boolean algebra—a general way of analyzing the grammar of all possible relations between ideas--that enabled the cognitive revolution and the information revolution. You don’t need to read Boole’s Laws of Thought to get why this is important (although, please do). The fact that you are reading this on a computer, that relies for its very existence on the accuracy of his analysis, is the actual pudding whose juicy proof you are eating. Or rather, reading.

Thus, it is simply factually inaccurate to claim that modern cognitive science is drawing on the most impressive current technology to explain thought. In term of historical progression, it was Boole’s attempts to fully analyze thought that led to the creation, long after his death, of the technology. It was what made the technology possible in the first place--by giving cognition functional analysis. Functionalism is the thread that runs through all science. It’s the insight that essentialism--belief in magic properties--is a blind alley. We know better. Functionalism is the stance that what something is, is what it does. Functionalism about thought—that minds are what brains do--came a hundred years before the technology that was the triumphant vindication of that insight.

THE BASICS

  • What Is Cognition?
  • Find a therapist near me

[And the path from Boole via Shannon, Turing, and von Neumann to the modern understanding of information and order is a deep and interesting one marrying together information theory and physics. I simply don't have time or space for it here]

Thinking is, as thinking does

So much for history. But it segues neatly into the second point that Epstein fails to appreciate. Cognitive scientists (unless they are very confused) do not think that human minds are computers. Rather—they think that computers—the physical objects on your desk top say—are just one way to make functions real. Functions are mathematical operations—but we shouldn’t get too hung up on numbers and equations here. You can turn an equation into a physical object yourself (and back again) and you did it in high school (you called it “making a graph”) and there are many ways to see that functions can be realized in different ways.

Machines in general are ways to make abstracted functions into physical things. And that’s what cognitive scientists study. Functions. E.g. “How do we turn electromagnetic inputs into perceptions?” or “How do our past experiences function to make us wary of similar present dangers?”. Epstein's frustration that cognitive scientists keep on thinking this way is simply misplaced. Hilariously he offers what he thinks is an alternative to functional thinking in explaining catching a ball:

Cognition Essential Reads

5 Barriers to Critical Thinking

Why Chimpanzees Can't Learn Language: 1

“The IP [Information Processing] perspective requires the player to formulate an estimate of various initial conditions of the ball’s flight – the force of the impact, the angle of the trajectory, that kind of thing – then to create and analyze an internal model of the path along which the ball will likely move, then to use that model to guide and adjust motor movements continuously in time in order to intercept the ball.

That is all well and good if we functioned as computers do, but McBeath and his colleagues gave a simpler account: to catch the ball, the player simply needs to keep moving in a way that keeps the ball in a constant visual relationship with respect to home plate and the surrounding scenery (technically, in a ‘linear optical trajectory’). This might sound complicated, but it is actually incredibly simple, and completely free of computations, representations and algorithms.”

Now this raises an interesting challenge. Namely: Build something that performs this "simple task". From scratch. No helping yourself to pre-existing (functional) systems that have already solved those problems of alignment and integrated movement that you so cavalierly wave your hand around. I'll watch and chuckle. Epstein has made the classic mistake (and especially egregious for a psychologist) in thinking that what appears simple to conscious access does not have a wealth of highly complex unconscious processing going on beneath the surface. The brilliant Hans Morovec gave his name to the general error of this kind: Morovec’s paradox. (4)

Morovec asked the question: Why is it that it takes the smartest humans to do things like fly planes, diagnose disease, and play chess; when we can make fairly stupid computers that beat all but the best of humans at these things with ease? The other side of this coin is that things that we (wrongly) thought would be computationally easy to program—like walking up stairs, recognizing faces, and so on turned out to be horribly difficult to get computers to do. Why was this? We were making the same mistake as Epstein—forgetting that evolutionarily novel tasks (like chess) have their computational architecture laid bare. People who think a task is easy simply haven’t given serious thought to the millions of years that went into making it so. That’s why we need smart people to do things like play chess, because there isn’t that much (in computational terms) to know (and the humans with the biggest brains know it better and faster). As the brilliant AI technician Rob Brooks pointed out, “Elephants don’t play chess”. Learning to be an elephant took millions of years prior to that particular elephant’s existence. Learning chess takes a few years. (5)

The alternative to omniscience isn't despair

Epstein gets more and more frustrated with the benighted community of cognitive scientists as he goes on.

“To understand even the basics of how the brain maintains the human intellect, we might need to know not just the current state of all 86 billion neurons and their 100 trillion interconnections, not just the varying strengths with which they are connected, and not just the states of more than 1,000 proteins that exist at each connection point, but how the moment-to-moment activity of the brain contributes to the integrity of the system.”

Or—you could just listen to what that brain is saying? Through its attached mouth, for example? If someone was to object that what I’ve just said in response is a trick—that you don’t know every single thing that that particular brain is doing at any particular moment the appropriate response is: “So what?” I don’t know what every single one of the 10^26 sub-atomic particles in my cup of coffee are doing on an individual basis either. But I know what they are doing on the aggregate as I pick up the cup. This is called “heat”. And it’s the mean kinetic energy of the molecules in the liquid (e.g. the average amount of whizzing about they are doing—and good luck trying to map those on an individual basis). I don’t need to know everything in order to know anything.

We still have many problems to solve—but they are problems, not mysteries. And there is only one game in town to solve them: Functionalism. The alternative that Epstein offers—essentialism--has gone the way of astrology, alchemy, and homeopathy. And for the exact same reason. Essentialism comes from the pre-science time of humans. It’s magical thinking.

There is a saying that those who think that a task is impossible should not get in the way of those achieving it. The irony is that the opponents of cognitive science live in a world where airplanes fly themselves, machines govern investments, artificial eyes and hands can be spliced into the place of lost ones, directly into nervous systems. These things work because functionalism is true. Boole was right all along. Your mind isn't a computer: it's thousands of them. The functionalist account of the human brain isn’t something we are predicting. We are living in the midst of it. (6)

Comic strip by Randall Monroe, from XKCD.com (CC BY-NC 2.5).

Thinking About Thinking (2024)

FAQs

How do I stop thinking about thinking? ›

Techniques, including deep breathing and asking for help from a healthcare professional, may help alleviate the stress of overthinking.
  1. Destructive thought patterns. ...
  2. Step back and look at how you're responding. ...
  3. Find a distraction. ...
  4. Take a deep breath. ...
  5. Meditate. ...
  6. Look at the bigger picture. ...
  7. Do something nice for someone else.

Why is it important to think about our thinking? ›

Metacognition is a big word for something most of us do every day without even noticing: Thinking about our own thoughts. Reflecting on our thoughts is a big part of understanding our feelings and learning new things.

Why do I have trouble saying what I'm thinking? ›

It's a symptom of damage to the parts of the brain that control language. The signs of aphasia depend on which part of the brain is damaged. There are four main types of aphasia: Expressive aphasia is when you know what you want to say, but you have trouble saying or writing your thoughts.

What does are you thinking what I'm thinking mean? ›

Not coincidentally, “Are you thinking what I'm thinking” has become one of my younger son Maxwell's favorite phrases lately. It means he has an idea of something fun to do and is hoping you have the same thing in mind.

Is overthinking a form of anxiety? ›

No, overthinking isn't a recognized mental health condition, but it can be a symptom of depression or anxiety. Overthinking is commonly associated with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), says Duke. GAD is characterized by the tendency to worry excessively about several things.

Am I overthinking or am I right? ›

Signs that you might be overthinking include: Dwelling on past events or situations. Second-guessing decisions you've made. Replaying your mistakes in your mind.

What is thinking about thinking called? ›

Metacognition is the process of thinking about thinking. It's about examining how we take in and process information, and figuring out ways we can do that more efficiently.

Why do I think about thinking so much? ›

Overthinking can be an early indicator or symptom of depression, anxiety, and other mental health conditions. To stop overthinking, you can try challenging your thoughts, reaching out to loved ones for support, or talking to a mental health professional for extra help.

What is an example of thinking about thinking? ›

Here are some examples of metacognition: A student learns about what things help him or her to remember facts, names, and events. A student learns about his or her own style of learning. A student learns about which strategies are most effective for solving problems.

What is it called when you can't articulate your thoughts? ›

Dysgraphia and expressive language disorder both affect language use and learning. Dysgraphia can make it hard to put thoughts in writing. Expressive language disorder can make it hard to express thoughts and ideas when speaking and writing. (You may hear it called a “language disorder” or a “communication disorder.”)

Why can't my brain think of words to say? ›

Aphasia is when a person has difficulty with their language or speech. It's usually caused by damage to the left side of the brain (for example, after a stroke).

Why do I struggle to think of words when talking? ›

Depending on which areas of the brain are affected, a person can have an expressive aphasia, a receptive aphasia, or both. The most common causes of aphasia are stroke, traumatic brain injury, neurodegenerative conditions, brain tumors, and epilepsy.

What is the difference between I'm thinking about you and I'm thinking of you? ›

“I'm thinking of you” is usually sweet, sometimes romantic. It can mean “thinking of you often” or “thinking of you right now.” It sounds like you wish them well. “I'm thinking about you” is often heavier, sometimes (not always!) creepy or threatening.

Is the person I'm thinking about thinking about me? ›

However, as of now, scientifically speaking, it does not mean that they are thinking about you, too. While it is a romantic thought, it is not psychologically proven that your thoughts are in any way connected to the other person's thoughts.

How do you tell I was thinking about you? ›

You've been on my mind a lot lately.” “Thinking of you always makes my day.” “Just wanted to send some happy thoughts your way today.”

Why is it important to think carefully about critical thinking? ›

It plays a pivotal role in enhancing decision-making and problem-solving processes. By employing critical thinking, individuals can evaluate information objectively, consider multiple perspectives, and analyze complex situations more effectively.

What are the benefits of thinking more? ›

Health benefits that positive thinking may provide include:
  • Increased life span.
  • Lower rates of depression.
  • Lower levels of distress and pain.
  • Greater resistance to illnesses.
  • Better psychological and physical well-being.
  • Better cardiovascular health and reduced risk of death from cardiovascular disease and stroke.

How does thinking benefit you? ›

The health benefits of positive thinking may include:

Greater resistance to the common cold. Better coping skills during stressful times. Better psychological and physical well-being. Reduced risk of death from cardiovascular disease.

How does thinking affect your life? ›

Thoughts drive your emotions, 'what you think you become' – Guatama Buddha. When your thoughts appear to be the product of your overwhelming sadness and grief, know that it is your thoughts that are feeding the sadness rather than the other way around. Your thoughts generate a feeling which you then act upon.

References

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Rubie Ullrich

Last Updated:

Views: 5900

Rating: 4.1 / 5 (72 voted)

Reviews: 87% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Rubie Ullrich

Birthday: 1998-02-02

Address: 743 Stoltenberg Center, Genovevaville, NJ 59925-3119

Phone: +2202978377583

Job: Administration Engineer

Hobby: Surfing, Sailing, Listening to music, Web surfing, Kitesurfing, Geocaching, Backpacking

Introduction: My name is Rubie Ullrich, I am a enthusiastic, perfect, tender, vivacious, talented, famous, delightful person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.